bird data > past walk reports

3/26/12

We had high hopes as we set out on a beautiful, cloudy day, just a day after a rain that had blanketed the area. Darren said he would not be satisfied with anything less than 30 bird species. Darren wound up not satisfied. We observed 25 species, which is 5 above the median for a week 13 but way below the maximum, 37, which is also the maximum observed for any week of the year.

See the plots at http://birdwalks.caltech.edu/bird_data/species_time.html and http://birdwalks.caltech.edu/bird_data/two_plots.htm

The hightlight for me was the little flock of Chipping Sparrows seen in the maintenance yard. This is only the second time this bird species has been recorded on the walks. The first time was earlier this year. So that was neat.

The other neat thing was the Common Yellowthroat sighting. It was a brilliant male perched on a big boulder. Earlier in the day I had seen a female or a juvenile so there is more than one of this species on campus. Neat.

The date: 3/26/2012
The week number: 13
The walk number: 1136
The weather: 60°F, partly cloudy

The walkers: Alan Cummings, Darren Dowell, Matt Bradford, Vicky Brennan, Viveca Sapin-Areeda, Kent Potter, Tom Palfrey

The birds (25):

House Sparrow
Mourning Dove
House Finch
Anna's Hummingbird
American Crow
Yellow-rumped Warbler
European Starling
Lesser Goldfinch
Band-tailed Pigeon
Snowy Egret
Red-shouldered Hawk
Dark-eyed Junco
Orange-crowned Warbler
Black Phoebe
Turkey Vulture
Chipping Sparrow
Nuttall's Woodpecker
House Wren
Ruby-crowned Kinglet
Red-whiskered Bulbul
Bushtit
Common Raven
Yellow-chevroned Parakeet
Hummingbird, Selasphorus
Common Yellowthroat

Respectfully submitted,
Alan Cummings
3/28/12

http://birdwalks.caltech.edu/index.html







3/19/12

The temperature was even cooler than last week and we had fewer birders, as Alan and Darren were both absent. The walk nevertheless began very well. Kent brought a snowy egret and the Maintenance yard was quite productive. We left with 17 species and I am thinking that we are going to bust through the thirty bird barrier, as we had in the preceding two weeks, but the playing field, track, and Tournament Park yielded almost nothing (a house sparrow that we were happy to get from the playing field and a band-tailed pigeon from Tournament Park). We managed to pick up a few species in the second half of the walk and got some very nice views of a small flock of western bluebirds (east side of Wilson near Mead), perhaps the same group we saw a couple of weeks ago. However, the numbers of new species advanced slowly. Some regulars like the yellow-chevroned parakeet that have been reliably ensconced at mid-day in one of the silk-floss trees outside the student services building on Holliston, appear to have moved on. Finally, we arrive at the Throop ponds, the official end of the walk. A snowy egret is foraging along the shore of the lower pond. He puts on a great show by catching a minnow but he doesn't add to the species total because Kent had seen him earlier. I don't hear or see the common yellowthroat but this wasn't unexpected given the lack of Alan's portable song contraption and my inability to hear him unless he is singing within two or three meters of me (this has happened a couple of times but not today). I walk up to the second pond, hoping to see our mallard on mallard rock but he isn't there. This leads to a lightly flushing annoyance over a drake's lack of consideration (he was there at 3 PM and again at noon on Tuesday) when the gelding thought evaporates in the extended chatter of a mountain chickadee, only a few meters away. We had our last bird of the day and 22 for the walk. This total is respectable and well above both minimum (12) and median (19) but the construct of expectation posed by the preceding two weeks of stellar species counts and an early great start made it somehow disappointing. I had allowed myself to be caught up in a numbers game.

See the plots at http://birdwalks.caltech.edu/bird_data/species_time.html and http://birdwalks.caltech.edu/bird_data/two_plots.htm

For the bird of the week, I would like to nominate the pair of American robins we saw in the Maintenance yard. The light was so harsh that the breasts appeared almost yellow at times but the birds were close enough so that pose, field markings, and ultimately the red breasts were all bringing internally consistent indicators of species. We also had a bird in bad light perched at the top of a tree. This bird was patient but did not provide a good look. After much looking, mulling, and consultation we went with the identification of a female hooded oriole and I wrote this down on the tracking list. However, the identification kept gnawing at me as the walk proceeded and, by the time we turned east off of Wilson, I had crossed it out. Hooded orioles are currently present in the area, though rare (most of them are coming through later in the Spring) but the id was shaky and, I think, ultimately not up to our standard level of confidence for calling a bird.

So, with the hooded oriole discarded, the bird of the week is the American robin. If you look at Alan's probability plot for this species, you will see two large masses of sightings, early and late in the year, which is consistent with a wintering bird that migrates away from Caltech in the Spring. The apparent duopoly is artificial because the wintering season cuts across the artificial boundary between successive calendar years. This becomes really apparent if you simply shift the x-scale and plot from weeks -25 to +26 (i.e., from late June in one year to late June in the next year) instead of weeks 1 to 52. This procedure yields a distribution with the bulk of the data looking like a reasonably good Gaussian (I haven't tested it) centered roughly around week 8 (i.e., late February) and a second, smaller, peak centered around week 42 (mid- to late October). My Fearless Fosdick suggestion is that the small peak is driven by general migration of robins passing through Caltech on their way here or to other wintering locales and that the large peak reflects local wintering birds whose daily track, which probably runs over a couple of kilometers, happens to intersect our route at just the right time. We don't see a migration peak in the Spring but I suppose that this may be a hidden shoulder in the large peak. We also don't see robins in the sumemr, which suggests that we don't have a lot of robin nesting activity on campus, although they are known to nest in the general area.

If you take a four year running average of American robin sightings to damp out some of the year-to-year variability and account for artificially splitting seasons by using calendar years, you get a cyclical variation with an apparent wavelength of about 14 years and minima around years 1995 and 2009. The late eighties peak was ten or eleven sightings per year versus seven or eight in the early two thousands, suggesting that our local population of robins experienced a decline in population from highs in the eighties, perhaps something as simple as shifts in preferred berry-bearing bushes. Robins are predominately frugivores (fruit eaters) in the Fall and winter, so much so, that they are viewed as agricultural pests in the berry industry (e.g., cherries, blueberries, and grapes). The minima in sightings per year are similar at about five, suggesting stabilization, but I worry that this may be compounded by a greater average number of birders on the walk increasing the probability of seeing a smaller number of robins. We see this phenomenon in the sightings of turkey vultures on the bird walk. The numbers have been increasing in recent years but this is because more people, on average, are looking for them.

If you are a virus and you want to generate a global pandemic in some species, you need a designated victim, a vector by which the disease is transmitted, and a super spreader, which need not be the same species as the intended victim. Ebola creates a very nasty deadly disease for humans but it's not, at least in its recent incarnations, a pandemic threat because it kills an infected human so fast and in such a horrible way that the victim has few chances to spread the disease to other humans. You can think of West Nile Virus (WNV) as a sort of Ebola for crows. If an infected mosquito (the vector) bites an uninfected crow, it transmits WNV to the crow. If a second uninfected mosquito then bites the now infected crow, it too will become a carrier. However, once infected, the crow tends to die quickly so that he has relatively few opportunities to propagate the disease through additional mosquito bites. Enter the robin. American robins are widespread and numerous in North America and they tend to roost communally. Mosquitoes prefer robins to other birds like sparrows (don't ask me why) and robins can tolerate WNV far better than crows. American robins are the super spreaders of WNV and this gives us the makings of a crow pandemic. So, if you don't like crows and view WNV as a way to drop the local crow population (you also have to not like other members of the crow family like jays and ravens), you should thank your lucky robins.

The date: 3/19/2012
The week number: 12
The walk number: 1135
The weather: 53°F, sunny

The walkers: John Beckett, Viveca Sapin-Areeda, Kent Potter, Vicky Brennan

The birds (22):

Northern Mockingbird
House Sparrow
Mourning Dove
House Finch
Anna's Hummingbird
American Crow
European Starling
Hummingbird, Selasphorus
Common Raven
Yellow-rumped Warbler
Snowy Egret
Western Bluebird
Red-whiskered Bulbul
Ruby-crowned Kinglet
Cooper's Hawk
Lesser Goldfinch
Spotted Towhee
Black Phoebe
American Robin
Band-tailed Pigeon
Bushtit
Mountain Chickadee

-- John Beckett

Respectfully submitted,
Alan Cummings
3/26/12

http://birdwalks.caltech.edu/index.html







3/12/12

The temperature was cooler than last week by 20 degrees but the birds were in great abundance and in a fine fettle. In the Maintenance yard, Darren was practically on fire, calling out species and waving in various directions faster than the rest of us could turn our heads but the visuals came eventually. Overall, we encountered 30 species and, had it not been for a 35 species walk in week 11 of last year (second best all time), we would have tied the record for the week.

See the plots at http://birdwalks.caltech.edu/bird_data/species_time.html and http://birdwalks.caltech.edu/bird_data/two_plots.htm

Although there was a fair amount of overlap in species with the walk last year and we had nine species that we didn't see then, the 2011 week 11 walk cleaned up on hawks (including a Swainson's), towhees, wrens, and warblers and we just couldn't match the collection. I have to admit that I would certainly like to have seen another Swainson's hawk but we have nothing to complain about. We had a very good walk.

I have come to think of thirty bird walks as a fairly standard signature of the Spring migration. It's a very good week when we hit thirty but it doesn't bear a histrionic caricature breasting decades. After all, we had 32 birds last week and, last year, we had five walks with thirty or more species. However, this shows a lack of context. From a historical perspective, 30+ species walks are extraordinary with only ten such walks all time (out of 1134 walks), all but one in the band of weeks nine to thirteen (the record dips to a mere 29 in weeks fourteen and fifteen), and all ten of these 30 species walks can be found in one of three years, 2006, 2011, and 2012. So, we are walking in lucky times.

Although I had thirty species to choose from, there was no obvious candidate for the bird of the week. Usually, I try to pick an unusual bird or an unusually good view of a bird but, this week, we didn't really have a reasonable candidate. Identifications were solid but not special. So, I will go with another of Melanie's new lifers, a decent but not spectacular look at a red-crowned parrot camped out on Wilson but not talking. Red-crowned parrots are the dominant Caltech area parrot right now but this hasn't always been the case. They've been in the San Gabriel Valley since at least the early 70s but we first encountered them on a walk in 1996. Between 1996 and 2006, we would see them in two pulses, centered around weeks 8 and 48. So, these were mostly Fall/winter birds for us. We didn't see red-crowned parrots at all between 2007 and 2009 but, we had seven sightings in 2010 and ten last year. The new pattern of sightings is, however, completely different from what it was a decade ago. Our red-crowned parrots are now Spring birds with a skewed peak cresting around week 20 and a drift to the odd sighting during the Summer. There has been a paradigm shift and the timing suggests that we now have a local breeding population that goes elsewhere after the nurseries break up. Since red-crowned parrots are secondary hole nesters, our local woodpeckers must be working overtime to accommodate the demand. We have two parrots (red-crowned and yellow-headed) and two parakeets (yellow-chevroned and red-masked) that currently frequent Caltech. Visually, the quick way to separate the parrots from the parakeets is through the tail. Parrots have relatively stubby tails and parakeet tails are pointy.

However, I want to discuss a different approach to identification. Each of these four species has a distinctive voice and, with a little practice, you can identify which one you are dealing with solely based on the sound. In fact, I would suggest that this grouping of four exotic feral avian species may be the best place to start building an auditory vocabulary for local birds. They are loud and, generally, not shy. They talk a lot, especially when in flocks, and, even better, I can actually hear them. So, how do you sort them out short of seeing a flock in action and, over time perhaps, learning to associate a particular call with a particular species? Generally, if you ask a birder about where to go to hear bird songs and calls, you are sent to the Cornell site (http://www.allaboutbirds.org). This is a well organized site specializing in American birds and you will get a typical song and, perhaps, a typical call. However, exotics like parrots and parakeets are not included in the Cornell site and you will, based on what this site has to offer, often find yourself with a slim collection of voice relative to what the bird of interest can produce, even if it happens to have your bird. You don't gain anything on this front if you have a subscription to the Birds of North America, which is also run through Cornell. It is fairly rich in information (I use it as a back stop for background on birds of the week) but does not have a value added vocal component. This is a problem for building an auditory vocabulary because many species of birds have flight calls, perching calls, songs for males, songs for females, assorted non-call noises and seasonal and regional dialects on all of the above. So, unless your Cornell bird happens to mesh with your local target, you may have a hard time getting a sense for either the variations or the commonalities.

An alternative site more suited for picking up some auditory expertise is (thank you George Rossman) xeno-canto (http://www.xeno-canto.org), which has an extensive collection of calls and songs, currently with representation for nearly 8000 species of birds (i.e., about 80% of all bird species). There are, just to pick something at "random" because I happened to have heard one the other day, 56 recordings of the white- breasted nuthatch. There are four recordings of red-crowned parrots (listed under red-crowned amazon), none for a yellow-headed amazon (you can't win them all but here are a couple of recordings if you want to pursue the species: http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=EmpFBIhYBgU and http://www.westernsoundscape.org/audio/yellow-headed-parrot-pasadena.mp3), 17 for red- masked parakeets and 21 for yellow-chevroned parakeets. Personally, I find going through a lot of examples of something, whether it be a visual or auditory problem, a much better way to get a sense for a bird species than memorizing one recording or image and hoping that you can stuff your bird into the same frame. That's a dangerous proposition if you have to do any judicious cropping and blending to make the species fit.

The date: 3/12/2012
The week number: 11
The walk number: 1134
The weather: 60F, partly cloudy

The walkers: Alan Cummings, Viveca Sapin-Areeda, John Beckett, Melanie Channon, Darren Dowell, Vicky Brennan

The birds (30):

Northern Mockingbird
House Sparrow
Mourning Dove
House Finch
Anna's Hummingbird
American Crow
Lesser Goldfinch
European Starling
Yellow-rumped Warbler
Mallard
Swift, Species
Orange-crowned Warbler
Cooper's Hawk
Dark-eyed Junco
Bushtit
Black Phoebe
Mountain Chickadee
American Goldfinch
Downy Woodpecker
Turkey Vulture
House Wren
Nuttall's Woodpecker
Ruby-crowned Kinglet
Common Raven
Red-whiskered Bulbul
Hummingbird, Selasphorus
Red-crowned Parrot
Band-tailed Pigeon
Yellow-chevroned Parakeet
Common Yellowthroat

-- John Beckett

Respectfully submitted,
Alan Cummings
3/16/12

http://birdwalks.caltech.edu/index.html







3/5/12

It was a warm day, nearly thirty degrees higher than the 54F of last Monday and the air seemed to exude a feeling of Spring, if not summer. I have to admit that my expectations were not high because of the unseasonably warm weather but the birds certainly knew the time. It is the time of El Norte. It is time, at least for the early birds, to move into breeding grounds. Our Say's phoebe is gone, off to the desert, but we had white-throated swifts swirling around Millikan and saw our first house wren of the year. In absence and presence, these are sure signs of Spring. Our common yellowthroat is still hanging around the Millikan ponds (actually the Indian hawthorns to the east of the ponds). He was our last bird of the walk and he capped a four warbler day. The common yellowthroat is in clear breeding plumage now, another sure sign of Spring. We got a western tanager, a dark-eyed junco, though not before complaining that we hadn't been seeing any, and an undeserved but happily accepted snowy egret. We also saw a couple of chipping sparrows, the first new species for the Caltech bird list since the great rock wren extravaganza of 2010 and we did fairly well with year-round residents. We ended, fittingly, with the common yellowthroat and 32 species, our first 30 plus species walk of the year, a new record species count for week 10, and our second new record for this year.

See the plots at http://birdwalks.caltech.edu/bird_data/species_time.html and http://birdwalks.caltech.edu/bird_data/two_plots.htm

Before mentioning the obvious highlight chipping sparrows, it seems appropriate to say something about the white-throated swifts. We have been seeing them off and on around town, especially near the Colorado Street bridge over the arroyo, but only rarely around Millikan and the ones that we have seen on campus but off walk didn't seem to want to stay with us long enough to be captured at lunch time. This batch of birds seems more serious, for some reason, and perhaps these are going to be nesting birds or, at least, persistent lunch time snackers. Over the last dozen years, week 11 is the average for the first appearance of white-throated swifts on the bird walk, so week 10 as the first sighting of the year is not atypical (also true of house wrens, incidentally).

The Caltech bird walk was started in 1986 and we have been seeing white-throated swifts since 1987. It was, however, not until 2004 that we started consistently bringing in more than half a dozen sightings a year (the running average has been six or more since 2000). Part of this may reflect scheduling conflicts (swifts' scheduling, not ours) that leaves the swifts out of visual/auditory range during the noon hour (we have this problem with starlings, easy to find at 8 AM and 5 PM but a hard catch at noon) and part of this may be due to the fact that the swifts didn't make Millikan a regular center of attention until several years ago, even though the library has been available for nesting and foraging since 1967. If you plot a four year running average of the number of sightings per year as a function of leading (i.e., most recent) year, you get a steady rise from 1994 to 2004, followed by a slow decline with a hint of another rise over the last couple of years.

Within a given year, we can generally count on seeing our first white-throated swifts sometime around week eleven, as noted above, and this is followed by a slow steady rise in probability, peaking around week 20 (mid-May), followed by a slow decline to week 30 (mid-July), consistent with local breeding and dispersal. There are additional pulses of sightings around weeks 32 (early August) and 38 (mid-September), which are likely associated with migrating swifts passing through on their way south, but we can have a swift sighting at any time of the year. One thing I like about swifts, in general, is that they do practically everything in the air. Obviously, they catch insects on the fly (they have small beaks but relatively large gapes so they can swallow a large insect without first landing somewhere to pummel it into submission or break it up into smaller pieces). White-throated swifts also preen in the air, take baths in the air (they hit the water on the fly to soak up the water), and court in the air with chasing and communal dives. The pair dives can be spectacular, sometimes dropping several hundred feet in the process. I have seen a pair hit the ground in one of these dives but, usually, they (always a male and a female) break up before a collision occurs; it's not known whether this is strictly a courtship ritual or if mating is involved. White-throated swifts have taken nicely to tall buildings, so urbanization is not necessarily a bad thing for them, and they have been lately expanding their breeding range northward, although overall numbers seem to be in decline.

So, we saw a couple chipping sparrows (after Darren heard them and tracked them down to a tree by the driveway running towards Tournament Park). You might not think of this as a shocking event but it is. This was the first ever occurrence for a chipping sparrow on the Caltech bird walk. The Caltech bird list now stands at 136 categories (specific species plus ambiguous catch alls like "hawk, species") and 121 separate species, not bad for a non Arboretum, non wetlands, thoroughly urban institutional setting.

I've found the lack of Caltech chipping sparrows to be an oddity for some time. I see this species at home (Sierra Madre) pretty consistently in the Spring and, at least in breeding plumage, these birds are hard to confuse with anything else. In the Spring, they will have a dark bill, a prominent rusty red cap with no medial strip, and a sharply defined, virtually definitive, dark eye streak with a whitish supercilium between the streak and cap. These are hard to miss features if you have a decent look at the bird, so it would seem that we must never have gotten a decent look at one over the last couple of decades. I'm told that chipping sparrows sound a lot like dark-eyed juncos. Dark-eyed juncos are not especially common for us, so we would certainly have tried to locate any talking bird that sounded even remotely like a dark-eyed junco. I don't think we missed any chipping sparrows because of auditory misidentification. It's a curiosity (I tend to put our lack of Costa's hummingbirds in the same cabinet). During Spring migration, chipping sparrows usually move in small flocks of a few to a few dozen, with periodic stops for refreshment. That's probably what we encountered, a small flock stopping at Caltech for the day before moving on. In the Fall, chipping sparrows tend to travel in large flocks and that's potentially easy to miss.

I'll leave you with a couple chipping sparrow nuggets. The first is that, although you would be correct in thinking that chipping sparrows eat mostly seeds, they also eat a lot of insects and spiders during the breeding season, a third or more of their diet (you can get similar observations for hummingbirds with respect to nectar/insect loadings in the diet). These are high energy snacks and are likely important for fledging success (a decline in house sparrows in Britain can be tied to a decline in the local spider population). A second tidbit is that chipping sparrows appear to be an example of taxonomic over-splitting of species. There are three named subspecies currently accepted by the American Ornithological Union but mitochondrial DNA shows no differences among them. That doesn't mean that there aren't genetic differences but it does suggest that a little suspicion may be in order and, naturally, that fits in with the Caltech way.

The date: 3/5/2012
The week number: 10
The walk number: 1133
The weather: 81F, partly cloudy

The walkers: Alan Cummings, John Beckett, Darren Dowell, Vicky Brennan, Viveca Sapin-Areeda

The birds (32):

Northern Mockingbird
House Sparrow
Mourning Dove
House Finch
Anna's Hummingbird
American Crow
Mallard
Red-shouldered Hawk
White-throated Swift
Yellow-rumped Warbler
Black Phoebe
Lesser Goldfinch
Ruby-crowned Kinglet
Orange-crowned Warbler
Nuttall's Woodpecker
Gull, species
House Wren
Common Raven
American Robin
Chipping Sparrow
Red-whiskered Bulbul
Dark-eyed Junco
Red-tailed Hawk
Bushtit
Western Tanager
Townsend's Warbler
Hummingbird, Selasphorus
Cooper's Hawk
Band-tailed Pigeon
Yellow-chevroned Parakeet
Snowy Egret
Common Yellowthroat

-- John Beckett

Respectfully submitted,
Alan Cummings
3/9/12

http://birdwalks.caltech.edu/index.html







current walk report / two time plots / probability plots / raw data